Search (35 results, page 2 of 2)

  • × author_ss:"Cole, C."
  1. Yi, K.; Beheshti, J.; Cole, C.; Leide, J.E.; Large, A.: User search behavior of domain-specific information retrieval systems : an analysis of the query logs from PsycINFO and ABC-Clio's Historical Abstracts/America: History and Life (2006) 0.00
    4.4555214E-4 = product of:
      0.0066832816 = sum of:
        0.0066832816 = weight(_text_:in in 197) [ClassicSimilarity], result of:
          0.0066832816 = score(doc=197,freq=8.0), product of:
            0.044469737 = queryWeight, product of:
              1.3602545 = idf(docFreq=30841, maxDocs=44218)
              0.032692216 = queryNorm
            0.15028831 = fieldWeight in 197, product of:
              2.828427 = tf(freq=8.0), with freq of:
                8.0 = termFreq=8.0
              1.3602545 = idf(docFreq=30841, maxDocs=44218)
              0.0390625 = fieldNorm(doc=197)
      0.06666667 = coord(1/15)
    
    Abstract
    The authors report the findings of a study that analyzes and compares the query logs of PsycINFO for psychology and the two history databases of ABC-Clio: Historical Abstracts and America: History and Life to establish the sociological nature of information need, searching, and seeking in history versus psychology. Two problems are addressed: (a) What level of query log analysis - by individual query terms, by co-occurrence of word pairs, or by multiword terms (MWTs) - best serves as data for categorizing the queries to these two subject-bound databases; and (b) how can the differences in the nature of the queries to history versus psychology databases aid in our understanding of user search behavior and the information needs of their respective users. The authors conclude that MWTs provide the most effective snapshot of user searching behavior for query categorization. The MWTs to ABC-Clio indicate specific instances of historical events, people, and regions, whereas the MWTs to PsycINFO indicate concepts roughly equivalent to descriptors used by PsycINFO's own classification scheme. The average length of queries is 3.16 terms for PsycINFO and 3.42 for ABC-Clio, which breaks from findings for other reference and scholarly search engine studies, bringing query length closer in line to findings for general Web search engines like Excite.
  2. Cole, C.: ¬A rebuttal of the book review of the book titled "The Consciousness' Drive: Information Need and the Search for Meaning" : mapping cognitive and document spaces (2020) 0.00
    4.4555214E-4 = product of:
      0.0066832816 = sum of:
        0.0066832816 = weight(_text_:in in 5656) [ClassicSimilarity], result of:
          0.0066832816 = score(doc=5656,freq=2.0), product of:
            0.044469737 = queryWeight, product of:
              1.3602545 = idf(docFreq=30841, maxDocs=44218)
              0.032692216 = queryNorm
            0.15028831 = fieldWeight in 5656, product of:
              1.4142135 = tf(freq=2.0), with freq of:
                2.0 = termFreq=2.0
              1.3602545 = idf(docFreq=30841, maxDocs=44218)
              0.078125 = fieldNorm(doc=5656)
      0.06666667 = coord(1/15)
    
    Content
    Vgl. die Rezension in: JASIST 71(2020) no.1, S.118-120 (Heidi Julien).
  3. Cole, C.: Shannon revisited : information in terms of uncertainty (1993) 0.00
    4.410741E-4 = product of:
      0.0066161114 = sum of:
        0.0066161114 = weight(_text_:in in 4069) [ClassicSimilarity], result of:
          0.0066161114 = score(doc=4069,freq=4.0), product of:
            0.044469737 = queryWeight, product of:
              1.3602545 = idf(docFreq=30841, maxDocs=44218)
              0.032692216 = queryNorm
            0.14877784 = fieldWeight in 4069, product of:
              2.0 = tf(freq=4.0), with freq of:
                4.0 = termFreq=4.0
              1.3602545 = idf(docFreq=30841, maxDocs=44218)
              0.0546875 = fieldNorm(doc=4069)
      0.06666667 = coord(1/15)
    
    Abstract
    Shannon's theory of communication is discussed from the point of view of his concept of uncertainty. It is suggested that there are two information concepts in Shannon, two different uncertainties, and at least two different entropy concepts. Information science focuses on the uncertainty associated with the transmission of the signal rather than the uncertainty associated with the selection of a message from a set of possible messages. The author believes the latter information concept, which is from the sender's point of view, has more to say to information science about what information is than the former, which is from the receiver's point of view and is mainly concerned with 'noise' reduction
  4. Large, A.; Beheshti, J.; Cole, C.: Information architecture for the Web : the IA matrix approach to designing children's portals (2002) 0.00
    4.410741E-4 = product of:
      0.0066161114 = sum of:
        0.0066161114 = weight(_text_:in in 1010) [ClassicSimilarity], result of:
          0.0066161114 = score(doc=1010,freq=4.0), product of:
            0.044469737 = queryWeight, product of:
              1.3602545 = idf(docFreq=30841, maxDocs=44218)
              0.032692216 = queryNorm
            0.14877784 = fieldWeight in 1010, product of:
              2.0 = tf(freq=4.0), with freq of:
                4.0 = termFreq=4.0
              1.3602545 = idf(docFreq=30841, maxDocs=44218)
              0.0546875 = fieldNorm(doc=1010)
      0.06666667 = coord(1/15)
    
    Abstract
    The article presents a matrix that can serve as a tool for designing the information architecture of a Web portal in a logical and systematic manner. The information architect begins by inputting the portal's objective, target user, and target content. The matrix then determines the most appropriate information architecture attributes for the portal by filling in the Applied Information Architecture portion of the matrix. The article discusses how the matrix works using the example of a children's Web portal to provide access to museum information.
  5. Spink, A.; Cole, C.: ¬A human information behavior approach to a philosophy of information (2004) 0.00
    4.410741E-4 = product of:
      0.0066161114 = sum of:
        0.0066161114 = weight(_text_:in in 837) [ClassicSimilarity], result of:
          0.0066161114 = score(doc=837,freq=4.0), product of:
            0.044469737 = queryWeight, product of:
              1.3602545 = idf(docFreq=30841, maxDocs=44218)
              0.032692216 = queryNorm
            0.14877784 = fieldWeight in 837, product of:
              2.0 = tf(freq=4.0), with freq of:
                4.0 = termFreq=4.0
              1.3602545 = idf(docFreq=30841, maxDocs=44218)
              0.0546875 = fieldNorm(doc=837)
      0.06666667 = coord(1/15)
    
    Abstract
    This paper outlines the relation between philosophy of information (PI) and human information behavior (HIB). In this paper, we first briefly outline the basic constructs and approaches of PI and HIB. We argue that a strong relation exists between PI and HIB, as both are exploring the concept of information and premise information as a fundamental concept basic to human existence. We then exemplify that a heuristic approach to PI integrates the HIB view of information as a cognitive human-initiated process by presenting a specific cognitive architecture for information initiation based on modular notion from HIB/evolutionary psychology and the vacuum mechanism from PI.
    Footnote
    Artikel in einem Themenheft: The philosophy of information
  6. Cole, C.: Calculating the information content of an information process for a domain expert using Shannon's mathematical theory of communication : a preliminary analysis (1997) 0.00
    3.7806356E-4 = product of:
      0.005670953 = sum of:
        0.005670953 = weight(_text_:in in 2051) [ClassicSimilarity], result of:
          0.005670953 = score(doc=2051,freq=4.0), product of:
            0.044469737 = queryWeight, product of:
              1.3602545 = idf(docFreq=30841, maxDocs=44218)
              0.032692216 = queryNorm
            0.12752387 = fieldWeight in 2051, product of:
              2.0 = tf(freq=4.0), with freq of:
                4.0 = termFreq=4.0
              1.3602545 = idf(docFreq=30841, maxDocs=44218)
              0.046875 = fieldNorm(doc=2051)
      0.06666667 = coord(1/15)
    
    Abstract
    Using Bertram Brookes fundamental equation, sets out a method for calculating the information content of an information process. The knowledge structure variables in the Brookes' equation are operationalized, following principles set out in Claude Shannon's mathematical theory of communication. The set of 'a priori' alternatives and the 'a priori' probabilities assigned to each member of the set by the person undergoing the information process is the operational definition of the variable K(S) from the fundamental equation, which represented the person's knowledge structure before the information process takes place. The set of the a posteriori alternatives and the revised probabilities assigned to each member of the set by the person undergoing the information process is the operational definition of the Brookes variable which is the person's knowledge structure after the information process take place. Gives an example of an information process from a recent archeological discovery
  7. Cole, C.; Leide, J.E.: Using the user's mental model to guide the integration of information space into information need (2003) 0.00
    3.1505295E-4 = product of:
      0.004725794 = sum of:
        0.004725794 = weight(_text_:in in 1237) [ClassicSimilarity], result of:
          0.004725794 = score(doc=1237,freq=4.0), product of:
            0.044469737 = queryWeight, product of:
              1.3602545 = idf(docFreq=30841, maxDocs=44218)
              0.032692216 = queryNorm
            0.10626988 = fieldWeight in 1237, product of:
              2.0 = tf(freq=4.0), with freq of:
                4.0 = termFreq=4.0
              1.3602545 = idf(docFreq=30841, maxDocs=44218)
              0.0390625 = fieldNorm(doc=1237)
      0.06666667 = coord(1/15)
    
    Abstract
    The study reported here tested the efficacy of an information retrieval system output summary and visualization scheme for undergraduates taking a Vietnam War history who were in Kuhlthau's Stage 3 of researching a history essay. The visualization scheme consisted of (a) the undergraduate's own visualization of his or her essay topic, drawn by the student an the bottom half of a sheet of paper, and (b) a visualization of the information space (determined by index term counting) an the tophalf of the same page. To test the visualization scheme, students enrolled in a Vietnam War history course were randomly assigned to either the visualization scheme group, who received a high recall search output, or the nonvisualization group, who received a high precision search output. The dependent variable was the mark awarded the essay by the course instructor. There was no significant difference between the mean marks for the two groups. We were pleasantly surprised with this result given the bad reputation of high recall as a practical search strategy. We hypothesize that a more proactive visualization system is needed that takes the student through the process of using the visualization scheme, including steps that induce student cognition about task-subject objectives.
  8. Cole, C.; Mandelblatt, B.; Stevenson, J.: Visualizing a high recall search strategy output for undergraduates in an exploration stage of researching a term paper (2002) 0.00
    3.1505295E-4 = product of:
      0.004725794 = sum of:
        0.004725794 = weight(_text_:in in 2575) [ClassicSimilarity], result of:
          0.004725794 = score(doc=2575,freq=4.0), product of:
            0.044469737 = queryWeight, product of:
              1.3602545 = idf(docFreq=30841, maxDocs=44218)
              0.032692216 = queryNorm
            0.10626988 = fieldWeight in 2575, product of:
              2.0 = tf(freq=4.0), with freq of:
                4.0 = termFreq=4.0
              1.3602545 = idf(docFreq=30841, maxDocs=44218)
              0.0390625 = fieldNorm(doc=2575)
      0.06666667 = coord(1/15)
    
    Abstract
    When accessing an information retrieval system, it has long been said that undergraduates who are in an exploratory stage of researching their essay topic should use a high recall search strategy; what prevents them from doing so is the information overload factor associated with showing the undergraduate a long list of citations. One method of overcoming information overload is summarizing and visualizing the citation list. This paper examines five summarization and visualization schemes for presenting information retrieval (IR) citation output, then discusses whether these schemes are appropriate for undergraduates and other domain novice users. We ask and answer four questions: (1) What is the message these schemes try to communicate and (2) is this message appropriate for domain novice users like undergraduates? (3) How do these schemes communicate their message and (4) is how they communicate the message appropriate for a domain novice? We conclude that (i) the most appropriate message for information space visualizations for domain novice users is associative thinking, and (ii) the message should be communicated with a standardized look that remains relatively constant over time so that the shape and form of the visualization can become familiar and thus useful to students as they navigate their way through the information space produced by a high recall search strategy.
  9. Cole, C.; Leide, J.E.; Large, A,; Beheshti, J.; Brooks, M.: Putting it together online : information need identification for the domain novice user (2005) 0.00
    3.1505295E-4 = product of:
      0.004725794 = sum of:
        0.004725794 = weight(_text_:in in 3469) [ClassicSimilarity], result of:
          0.004725794 = score(doc=3469,freq=4.0), product of:
            0.044469737 = queryWeight, product of:
              1.3602545 = idf(docFreq=30841, maxDocs=44218)
              0.032692216 = queryNorm
            0.10626988 = fieldWeight in 3469, product of:
              2.0 = tf(freq=4.0), with freq of:
                4.0 = termFreq=4.0
              1.3602545 = idf(docFreq=30841, maxDocs=44218)
              0.0390625 = fieldNorm(doc=3469)
      0.06666667 = coord(1/15)
    
    Abstract
    Domain novice users in the beginning stages of researching a topic find themselves searching for information via information retrieval (IR) systems before they have identified their information need. Pre-Internet access technologies adapted by current IR systems poorly serve these domain novice users, whose behavior might be characterized as rudderless and without a compass. In this article we describe a conceptual design for an information retrieval system that incorporates standard information need identification classification and subject cataloging schemes, called the INIIReye System, and a study that tests the efficacy of the innovative part of the INIIReye System, called the Associative Index. The Associative Index helps the user put together his or her associative thoughts-Vannevar Bush's idea of associative indexing for his Memex machine that he never actually described. For the first time, data from the study reported here quantitatively supports the theoretical notion that the information seeker's information need is identified through transformation of his/her knowledge structure (i.e., the seeker's cognitive map or perspective an the task far which information is being sought).
  10. Leide, J.E.; Large, A.; Beheshti, J.; Brooks, M.; Cole, C.: Visualization schemes for domain novices exploring a topic space : the navigation classification scheme (2003) 0.00
    3.1505295E-4 = product of:
      0.004725794 = sum of:
        0.004725794 = weight(_text_:in in 1078) [ClassicSimilarity], result of:
          0.004725794 = score(doc=1078,freq=4.0), product of:
            0.044469737 = queryWeight, product of:
              1.3602545 = idf(docFreq=30841, maxDocs=44218)
              0.032692216 = queryNorm
            0.10626988 = fieldWeight in 1078, product of:
              2.0 = tf(freq=4.0), with freq of:
                4.0 = termFreq=4.0
              1.3602545 = idf(docFreq=30841, maxDocs=44218)
              0.0390625 = fieldNorm(doc=1078)
      0.06666667 = coord(1/15)
    
    Abstract
    In this article and two other articles which conceptualize a future stage of the research program (Leide, Cole, Large, & Beheshti, submitted for publication; Cole, Leide, Large, Beheshti, & Brooks, in preparation), we map-out a domain novice user's encounter with an IR system from beginning to end so that appropriate classification-based visualization schemes can be inserted into the encounter process. This article describes the visualization of a navigation classification scheme only. The navigation classification scheme uses the metaphor of a ship and ship's navigator traveling through charted (but unknown to the user) waters, guided by a series of lighthouses. The lighthouses contain mediation interfaces linking the user to the information store through agents created for each. The user's agent is the cognitive model the user has of the information space, which the system encourages to evolve via interaction with the system's agent. The system's agent is an evolving classification scheme created by professional indexers to represent the structure of the information store. We propose a more systematic, multidimensional approach to creating evolving classification/indexing schemes, based on where the user is and what she is trying to do at that moment during the search session.
  11. Beheshti, J.; Cole, C.; Abuhimed, D.; Lamoureux, I.: Tracking middle school students' information behavior via Kuhlthau's ISP Model : temporality (2015) 0.00
    3.1505295E-4 = product of:
      0.004725794 = sum of:
        0.004725794 = weight(_text_:in in 1819) [ClassicSimilarity], result of:
          0.004725794 = score(doc=1819,freq=4.0), product of:
            0.044469737 = queryWeight, product of:
              1.3602545 = idf(docFreq=30841, maxDocs=44218)
              0.032692216 = queryNorm
            0.10626988 = fieldWeight in 1819, product of:
              2.0 = tf(freq=4.0), with freq of:
                4.0 = termFreq=4.0
              1.3602545 = idf(docFreq=30841, maxDocs=44218)
              0.0390625 = fieldNorm(doc=1819)
      0.06666667 = coord(1/15)
    
    Abstract
    The article reports a field study investigating the temporality of the information behavior of 44 grade 8 students from initiation to completion of their school inquiry-based history project. The conceptual framework for the study is Kuhlthau's 6-stage information-search process (ISP) model. The objective of the study is to test and extend ISP model concepts. As per other ISP model studies, our study measured the evolution of the feelings, thoughts, and actions of the study participants over the 3-month period of their class project. The unique feature of this study is the unlimited access the researchers had to a real-life history class, resulting in 12 separate measuring periods. We report 2 important findings of the study. First, through factor analysis, we determined 5 factors that define the temporality of completing an inquiry-based project for these grade 8 students. The second main finding is the importance of the students' consultations with their classmates, siblings, parents, and teachers in the construction of the knowledge necessary to complete their project.
  12. Cole, C.: Operationalizing the notion of information as a subjective construct (1994) 0.00
    3.1188648E-4 = product of:
      0.004678297 = sum of:
        0.004678297 = weight(_text_:in in 7747) [ClassicSimilarity], result of:
          0.004678297 = score(doc=7747,freq=2.0), product of:
            0.044469737 = queryWeight, product of:
              1.3602545 = idf(docFreq=30841, maxDocs=44218)
              0.032692216 = queryNorm
            0.10520181 = fieldWeight in 7747, product of:
              1.4142135 = tf(freq=2.0), with freq of:
                2.0 = termFreq=2.0
              1.3602545 = idf(docFreq=30841, maxDocs=44218)
              0.0546875 = fieldNorm(doc=7747)
      0.06666667 = coord(1/15)
    
    Abstract
    We discuss information by attempting to operationalize it using: (1) Dervin and Nilan's idea that information is a subjective construct rather than an objective thing; (2) Brookes's idea that information is that which modifies knowledge structure; and (3) Neisser's idea that perception is top-down or schemata driven to the point of paradoxon. De Mey, Minsky's theorem of frames, and top-down and bottom-up models from reading theory are discussed. We conclude that information must be rare because only rare information can modify knowledge structure at its upper levels, and that to modify knowledge structure at its upper levels (its essence) information may have to enter the perception cycle in 2 stages
  13. Kennedy, L.; Cole, C.; Carter, S.: Connecting online search strategies and information needs : a user-centered, focus-labeling approach (1997) 0.00
    3.1188648E-4 = product of:
      0.004678297 = sum of:
        0.004678297 = weight(_text_:in in 627) [ClassicSimilarity], result of:
          0.004678297 = score(doc=627,freq=2.0), product of:
            0.044469737 = queryWeight, product of:
              1.3602545 = idf(docFreq=30841, maxDocs=44218)
              0.032692216 = queryNorm
            0.10520181 = fieldWeight in 627, product of:
              1.4142135 = tf(freq=2.0), with freq of:
                2.0 = termFreq=2.0
              1.3602545 = idf(docFreq=30841, maxDocs=44218)
              0.0546875 = fieldNorm(doc=627)
      0.06666667 = coord(1/15)
    
    Abstract
    When assisting undergraduate students in accessing materials, academic librarians must balance the task of conducting reference interviews with instructing students on how to use databases (OPACs, CD-ROM and online databases). Presents a method for connecting these tasks via the construction of a search strategy which is wholly dependent on the user's information needs. Using this method, the librarian assesses and explicitly labels the student's information need (using a diagnostoc tool based on Kuhlthau's and Taylor's concept of 'focus'), then assigns the most appropriate online search strategy for the satisfaction of this need
  14. Leide, J.E.; Cole, C.; Beheshti, J.; Large, A.; Lin, Y.: Task-based information retrieval : structuring undergraduate history essays for better course evaluation using essay-type visualizations (2007) 0.00
    2.2277607E-4 = product of:
      0.0033416408 = sum of:
        0.0033416408 = weight(_text_:in in 460) [ClassicSimilarity], result of:
          0.0033416408 = score(doc=460,freq=2.0), product of:
            0.044469737 = queryWeight, product of:
              1.3602545 = idf(docFreq=30841, maxDocs=44218)
              0.032692216 = queryNorm
            0.07514416 = fieldWeight in 460, product of:
              1.4142135 = tf(freq=2.0), with freq of:
                2.0 = termFreq=2.0
              1.3602545 = idf(docFreq=30841, maxDocs=44218)
              0.0390625 = fieldNorm(doc=460)
      0.06666667 = coord(1/15)
    
    Abstract
    When domain novices are in C.C. Kuhlthau's (1993) Stage 3, the exploration stage of researching an assignment, they often do not know their information need; this causes them to go back to Stage 2, the topic-selection stage, when they are selecting keywords to formulate their query to an Information Retrieval (IR) system. Our hypothesis is that instead of going backward, they should be going forward toward a goal state-the performance of the task for which they are seeking the information. If they can somehow construct their goal state into a query, this forward-looking query better operationalizes their information need than does a topic-based query. For domain novice undergraduates seeking information for a course essay, we define their task as selecting a high-impact essay structure which will put the students' learning on display for the course instructor who will evaluate the essay. We report a study of first-year history undergraduate students which tested the use and effectiveness of "essay type" as a task-focused query-formulation device. We randomly assigned 78 history undergraduates to an intervention group and a control group. The dependent variable was essay quality, based on (a) an evaluation of the student's essay by a research team member, and (b) the marks given to the student's essay by the course instructor. We found that conscious or formal consideration of essay type is inconclusive as a basis of a task-focused query-formulation device for IR.
  15. Cole, C.: ¬A theory of information need for information retrieval that connects information to knowledge (2011) 0.00
    2.2277607E-4 = product of:
      0.0033416408 = sum of:
        0.0033416408 = weight(_text_:in in 4474) [ClassicSimilarity], result of:
          0.0033416408 = score(doc=4474,freq=2.0), product of:
            0.044469737 = queryWeight, product of:
              1.3602545 = idf(docFreq=30841, maxDocs=44218)
              0.032692216 = queryNorm
            0.07514416 = fieldWeight in 4474, product of:
              1.4142135 = tf(freq=2.0), with freq of:
                2.0 = termFreq=2.0
              1.3602545 = idf(docFreq=30841, maxDocs=44218)
              0.0390625 = fieldNorm(doc=4474)
      0.06666667 = coord(1/15)
    
    Abstract
    This article proposes a theory of information need for information retrieval (IR). Information need traditionally denotes the start state for someone seeking information, which includes information search using an IR system. There are two perspectives on information need. The dominant, computer science perspective is that the user needs to find an answer to a well-defined question which is easy for the user to formulate into a query to the system. Ironically, information science's best known model of information need (Taylor, 1968) deems it to be a "black box"-unknowable and nonspecifiable by the user in a query to the information system. Information science has instead devoted itself to studying eight adjacent or surrogate concepts (information seeking, search and use; problem, problematic situation and task; sense making and evolutionary adaptation/information foraging). Based on an analysis of these eight adjacent/surrogate concepts, we create six testable propositions for a theory of information need. The central assumption of the theory is that while computer science sees IR as an information- or answer-finding system, focused on the user finding an answer, an information science or user-oriented theory of information need envisages a knowledge formulation/acquisition system.